|
||||
Home | Archives | About | Login | Submissions | Notify | Contact | Search | ||||
Copyright © 2007 by the author(s). Published here under license by The Resilience Alliance. Go to the pdf version of this article The following is the established format for referencing this article: Wittern, A. K., and Å. Berggren. 2007. Natal dispersal in the North Island robin (Petroica longipes): the importance of connectivity in fragmented habitats. Avian Conservation and Ecology - Écologie et conservation des oiseaux 2(2): 2. [online] URL: http://www.ace-eco.org/vol2/iss2/art2/ Research Papers Natal Dispersal in the North Island Robin (Petroica longipes): the Importance of Connectivity in Fragmented Habitats Dispersion natale chez le Miro de Garnot (Petroica longipes) : importance de la connectivité dans les habitats fragmentés 1Queen's University Belfast, 2Swedish University of Agricultural Sciences
Natal dispersal is an important component in bird population dynamics and can influence the persistence of local and metapopulations. We examined natal dispersal in the North Island robin (Petroica longipes), a sedentary bird species distributed in a fragmented forest habitat on Tiritiri Matangi Island, New Zealand. Earlier studies have shown that the only dispersal phase in this species takes place when juveniles leave their natal patch, and that juveniles who fail to find suitable habitat do not survive their first winter. These findings suggest that natal dispersal behavior in this species is important for population viability. We found that juveniles were highly affected by the fragmentation of the forest habitat, with patch occupancy being positively correlated with degree of connectivity of the landscape. Most juvenile movements (52.1%) were observed between patches that were separated by less than 20 m. Juvenile North Island robins were found in all forest habitat types, including young and open stands. This suggests that the juveniles are not dependent on old forest stands during their dispersal phase. Based on these findings, we suggest that management of this regionally-threatened species should focus not only on maintaining populations in occupied patches and increasing the habitat quality of these patches, but also on protecting existing forest patches acting as corridors and creating new forest habitat among patches. This would greatly increase the viability of the species’ metapopulations by increasing dispersal success between both unoccupied patches and subpopulations. Additionally, increased connectivity between forest patches could also be expected to increase the probability of successful dispersal of other threatened native species, many of which are also sensitive to the high degree of fragmentation of their habitats. Key words: natal dispersal; connectivity; habitat quality; intraspecific competition; habitat use; fragmentation; corridors; dispersal order; isolation; Petroica longipes Habitat fragmentation is a major cause of population decline and species extinction (Bender et al. 1998, Hames et al. 2001, Barbosa and Marquet 2002, Smith and Hellmann 2002). Fragmentation affects the spatial distribution of populations and may create a metapopulation structure in which subpopulations occupying remnant patches are linked only through the dispersal of individuals (Opdam 1991, Travis and Dytham 1998, Fleishman et al. 2002). The long-term survival of a metapopulation and its associated subpopulations is dependent on the interpatch dispersal of individuals, which increases genetic diversity in the different subpopulations and allows recolonization after local extinction (Charlesworth and Charlesworth 1987, Hanski 1999). The probability of interpatch dispersal in a fragmented landscape is a function of the behavioral or physical ability of a species to disperse through the habitat between the fragments (Ims 1995) as well as the size and isolation of the patch (Hansson 1991, Hames et al. 2001, Smith and Hellmann 2002). With reduced connectivity between habitat patches, and thus reduced movement of individuals, each subpopulation is at a higher risk of going extinct (Saunders et al. 1991). Natal dispersal is one of the primary factors moving individuals between subpopulations within a metapopulation. This is the process by which juveniles move away from their natal areas in search of a space in which they can reproduce (Miller et al. 1997, Kennedy and Ward 2003). This dispersal process may be initiated by intraspecific competition (Lambin et al. 2001) and may have evolved as a way to avoid inbreeding (Perrin and Goudet 2001). The success of natal dispersers can have a large impact on subpopulations as well as on the growth and persistence of the metapopulation as a whole (Withey and Marzluff 2005). Conspecific interaction is one of the major factors driving natal dispersal (Matthysen 2005) because of its influence on the initiation, length, and success of the dispersal phase. Because the occupants of an area will defend it against intruders, every interaction between a dispersing animal and the holder of a territory may prolong the dispersal phase (Armstrong 1995, Lambin et al. 2001). In a habitat patch with a high density of conspecifics and a high frequency of aggressive intraspecific interactions, a higher proportion of dispersing juveniles will be forced to continue searching for a territory, compared with juveniles that encounter a patch with a lower density of conspecifics (Sutherland et al. 2002). The stress from conspecific interactions for the natal dispersers may be further increased by reduced habitat availability and longer dispersal distances because of habitat fragmentation. This accumulative effect should be most severe when the species in focus is a poor disperser and the habitat is highly fragmented. Much of New Zealand has been deforested, and many of the remaining lowland forests are fragmented and isolated (Myers 1923, McGlone et al. 2005). Despite intensive conservation work and management involving native New Zealand birds (McLennan and Potter 1992, Innes et al. 2004), many species exist in a metapopulation structure because of habitat fragmentation. The North Island robin (Petroica longipes) is one of these species. A forest bird endemic to New Zealand’s North Island, this species declined dramatically during the 1900s because of habitat loss and the introduction of exotic mammalian predators such as rats, stoats, and cats. The robin is a sedentary and highly territorial species in which adult birds are reported to have poor dispersal abilities and to move over only short distances (Higgins and Peter 2003). Juvenile North Island robins have generally been reported to move only short distances as well, but some studies found movement over several kilometers (Oppel and Beaven 2004). However, there is little detailed data on natal dispersal behavior and habitat use in fragmented areas. It is known that juveniles that do not find an area to occupy during their first winter do not survive (Armstrong et al. 2000, Berggren et al. 2004). Consequently, natal dispersal in the robin is a crucial stage in the life of the individual and for the dynamics of the population. We studied natal dispersal in a population of North Island robins living in the fragmented forest of Tiritiri Matangi Island, east of the Whangaparoa peninsula in the North Island of New Zealand. In our study we examined the effects of landscape and intraspecific factors on juvenile dispersal behavior to address the following questions.
Study site Tiritiri Matangi Island (36°36’S, 174°53’E) is a 220-ha nature reserve located 3.5 km offshore and 28 km north of Auckland, New Zealand. The island was originally covered by broadleaf forest that was largely cleared for farming by Maori and European settlers. In 1970, when farming stopped, only 13 ha of forest remained in fragmented, isolated patches (Armstrong et al. 2002). Extensive replanting of native forest was undertaken between 1983 and 2002, and today forest patches of varying sizes (0.018–9.2 ha) cover 60% of the island’s area (Fig.1). Thus, the age of current forest patches on the island varies from recently planted (2–23 yr) to the old remnant stands spared by the early farmers. Although some forest patches are linked via habitat corridors or stepping-stone habitats, many are still surrounded by a matrix of grasslands that may contain solitary trees, most commonly the cabbage tree (Cordyline australis), and areas of flax (Phormium tenax). Study species The North Island robin is a small (26–32 g), endemic, sedentary forest bird found on New Zealand’s North Island. It was once distributed across the entire North Island, but its range has decreased dramatically in the last decades (Fleming 1950, Higgins and Peter 2003). Today the species is patchily distributed in the center of the North Island and on a few offshore islands (Higgins and Peter 2003). The conservation status of the New Zealand robin is “regionally threatened,” and it is currently not listed in any IUCN threat category. This classification is based on the assumption that the North Island robin is a subspecies, and does not take into account morphological and distributional data that suggest that the North Island and South Island robins (Petroica australis) are two separate species (Holdaway et al. 2001). Earlier studies have shown that robins prefer old forest to regenerating forest, indicating that territories in older forest areas are of higher quality than areas with younger forest (Armstrong and Ewen 2002). Because robins usually remain in the same territory until they die, individuals of this species tend to be highly sedentary once they have settled after the juvenile dispersal phase (Armstrong et al. 2000). The breeding season begins in September and ends in January; pairs can lay up to three clutches per season. Eggs are incubated for 17–20 d, and juveniles fledge at about 17 d of age. Parental food provisioning decreases approximately 30 d after fledging, and at this time juvenile robins begin leaving their parents’ territory to find their own breeding territory (Berggren 2006). The main predators of the North Island robin are the native owl (Ninox novaeseelandie) from the interior of the forest and the Australasian harrier (Circus approximans) from the surrounding matrix (Baker-Gabb 1981, Brown et al. 1998). Whereas owls hunt within the forest patch, harriers prey upon robins when they are visible through the canopy or when individuals cross the matrix between fragments; harriers have been seen circling close to canopy openings to localize robin song transmitted from recorders (A. Wittern and Å Berggren, personal observation). Hence, forest patches with little canopy cover may suffer increased predation risk. Also, for the robin as an open-cup nester, a more closed canopy would probably increase the likelihood that chicks would survive heavy rains and storms. The latter is supported by findings of dead chicks in exposed nests after heavy rain and cold weather (A. Witttern and Å. Berggren, personal observation). Study population During this study in the summer of 2003–2004, the size of the population of North Island robins on Tiritiri Matangi Island was 87 adults, with 74 of these in pairs. One hundred and ten juveniles were produced during the breeding season, and, with the ongoing banding scheme, 98% of the birds in the population were individually color-banded. Of the 110 robin fledglings produced during the breeding season, 34 juveniles were alive and observed dispersing before the end of the study in February 2004 and were incorporated into the analyses. Of these fledglings, two were foster-fed by unrelated males, a behavior previously observed in this species (Armstrong et al 2000, Berggren 2006). Because foster-fed juveniles also go through the natal dispersal phase, these individuals were included in the analyses. During the breeding season, the territorial boundaries, nesting sites, and breeding success of all the pairs in the population were recorded. The chicks were individually color-banded in the nest when they were between 9 and 15 d old. If a chick was not banded at this stage, it was later captured and banded while still in the parents’ territory. The capture was made using a hand net or a manually triggered spring trap after the juvenile was called in by a robin male song from a tape recorder and presented with mealworms, Tenebrio molitor (Armstrong 1995). Dispersal surveys Data on when juveniles became independent were collected by regularly visiting their natal territories at intervals of 1–5 d (see also below) around the time when they were estimated to start their dispersal, i.e., 30 d after fledging. When an exact dispersal date marking the day the juvenile left the parents’ territory was unknown, this date was estimated by using the median date between the day it was last seen in its natal territory and the date it was first seen outside this territory. To examine if the dispersal process was different depending on when during the season juveniles dispersed, juveniles were categorized as being “early” or “late” depending on whether they left their natal territory before or after 14 December, which was the median dispersal date in the study. To survey for birds outside their natal territories, researchers walked at a slow pace in a weaving pattern across the length and breadth of 51 forest fragments 0.018–9.2 ha in size (mean 1.5 ha ± 0.3) with stops at census points. Care was taken to spend an equal amount of time in each area to minimize the risk of that some areas might be more heavily surveyed than others. Census points had an in-between distance of 50 m. As an aid, we played a tape recording of a male robin’s song for 60 s, which attracted any robins in the vicinity. After the tape was played, we waited for 60 s and recorded any incoming birds. This survey technique has been very successful in previous studies, with a detection rate of greater than 90% (Armstrong and Ewen 2002). In the surveys we excluded 3.5% of the forested areas on the island because they were situated on steep cliffs facing the sea and were logistically difficult to work in (see Fig. 1). All forest patches were visited at least every 5th day. Every robin observed during surveys was identified by its band combination, and its location was determined using a Geographic Positioning System (GPS). If the density of the forest canopy prevented the GPS from accurately determining a position, the bird’s location was marked on an aerial photograph (scale 1:3760). All positions were transferred to digital maps using the GIS software Ozi-Explorer (2000). The maps were created from aerial photographs of the island and revegetation maps from the island’s revegetation project. These maps were controlled by ground-truthing to update data on vegetation and forest patch borders. On the resulting digitized maps, all North Island robin territory boundaries and all positions of individual birds were entered. Collection of habitat data To gather data on the habitats available for the dispersing juveniles, a detailed map of the island’s forest patches was produced. A set of transects were created in the forested areas with a 50-m distance between the lines. Depending on the size of the patch, one to nine transect lines crossed each fragment. At every 25 m along the lines, starting from the forest edge, a mapping site with a circle of 0.03 ha (a radius of 10 m) was drawn. Within this circle we classified the vegetation into one of four types: (1) closed stands of old forest, (2) open stands of old forest, (3) closed stands of young forest, and (4) open stands of young forest. The forest was classified as “old” if it consisted of old remnant stands spared by the early farmers, and “young” if it had been planted during the islands’ revegetation program. Data for the age of the forest were collected from historical vegetation maps and maps from the revegetation program. A stand was classified as “closed” if it had a canopy covering 50% or more of the area, and as “open” if canopy cover was less than 50%. One person (A. Wittern) did all the classification of the vegetation. We used the presence and absence of individuals of the species as the dependent variable to examine habitat use, a common procedure in short-term studies of landscape ecology (Opdam 1991). To gather presence and absence data from the different areas, both juveniles and adults were recorded along the transect lines. At every mapping site we recorded if a juvenile was seen or not. Adult robin presence was measured as the presence or absence of an adult territory at the mapping site. Forest patch characteristics and connectivity To investigate how the pattern of juvenile presence in a patch related to variables on the landscape and local scale, the following characteristics of the 51 censused forest patches were examined by extracting data from historical vegetation records and digitized maps: (1) mean forest age in the patch, (2) forest patch size, and (3) the distance between forest patches. The distance between forest patches was used to examine the effect of connectivity between forest fragments on two scales: (1) the local scale, or the distance from one patch to its nearest neighboring forest patch, and (2) the landscape scale, the mean distance from one patch to the five closest forest patches. The forest patches were then divided into five different categories showing different degrees of landscape connectivity: 1–20 m, 21–40 m, 41–60 m, 61–80 m, and > 80 m. Within each category, the juveniles observed were calculated as the percentage of the total number of juveniles observed in all patches. The counts were not individual based, i.e., a juvenile was counted as a new observation if it reached a new patch; however, a juvenile was counted only once in each patch. The availability of patches within each category was calculated as the percentage of the total number of patches examined. From this, the number of juveniles in each connectivity class that were expected to be observed if their distribution was random was calculated by dividing the number of observations by the proportion of patch availability. Analyses Habitat preference was analyzed with a chi-square goodness-of-fit test comparing the observed number of juveniles and adults with the number expected; the numbers expected were estimated by multiplying the proportion of the habitat type with the total number of observations of juveniles and adults at the respective sites. Only juveniles that left their natal patch were used in analyses. To determine which landscape variables were associated with the likelihood of a dispersing juvenile occurring in a given patch, we used ordinal logistic regression (McCollagh 1980). This statistical technique is useful when dealing with response data that do not conform to normal, Poisson, or binominal distributions (Thomson et al. 1998). The model consisted of four independent landscape variables: (1) mean forest age in the patch, (2) patch size, (3) local connectivity, and (4) landscape connectivity. The number of observed juveniles in the patch was used as the dependent ordinal variable. We used a backward elimination procedure of independent variables, progressively removing nonsignificant variables (partial P to remove = 0.10). Ordinal logistic regression consistently showed a good fit of the data, i.e., lack of fit with a P close to 1. Sample sizes varied among tests because some data could not be obtained for all areas. All analyses were carried out using JMP computer software (SAS 1999). Patch connectivity and forest age Of the four landscape variables tested, three had an impact on the number of juveniles that visited the patch: landscape connectivity, patch size, and mean forest age (Table 1, Fig 2A). A patch was more frequently visited during natal dispersal if it was more connected to other patches in the landscape. Also, the number of juveniles visiting the patch was positively correlated with the mean forest age of the patch and with patch size (Fig. 2A). Local connectivity was not significantly correlated to numbers of juveniles in the patch. When we examined the distribution of juveniles during their dispersal more closely, we found that 25 of the 48 counted juveniles (52.1%) were observed in patches with an isolation of less than 20 m, whereas only nine counts (18.8 %) were made in patches belonging to the three lowest connectivity classes with distances of 41–60, 61–80, and > 80 m, resulting in a very dissimilar distribution of observed and expected presences (chi-square test, χ² = 72.9, n = 48, P < 0.01; Fig. 2B). Dispersal time and behavior We found significant differences in the habitat type of forest patches in which early-dispersing juveniles were observed when compared to forest patches in which late-dispersing juveniles were seen. Patches occupied by juveniles dispersing early in the season, i.e., before 14 December, showed a higher degree of local connectivity [median and 95% CI early 3.0 m (3.0–3.0) , late 9.0 m (3.0–10.0), Mann Whitney U test; Z = 2.9, npatches early = 36, npatches late = 6, P = 0.004], and landscape connectivity (early 21.4 m ± 3.1, late 44.5 m ± 7.7, Mann Whitney U test; Z = 2.1, npatches early = 36, npatches late = 6, P = 0.038). Early dispersers tended to be found in bigger forest patches (5.0 ha ± 0.5) than were later dispersers (2.7 ha ± 1.3; Mann Whitney U test: Z = 1.9, npatches early = 36, npatches late = 6, P = 0.053). Also, the patches in which early-dispersing juveniles were found showed a higher total number of dispersing juveniles over the study period (early 4.3 ± 0.6, late 1.6 ± 1.4, Mann Whitney U test; Z = 2.1, npatches early = 27, npatches late = 5, P = 0.033). Forest patches in which early- and late-dispersing juveniles were observed did not differ in terms of age or the number of adult territories within them (Mann Whitney U test, all P > 0.15). Juvenile vs. adult habitat use The transect surveys in the 51 forest patches produced habitat data from 281 sites. Of the sites with known forest age, 70 (32.3%) were old forests or old, remnant stands and 147 (67.7%) were young, i.e., planted forests. Of the sites with old forest, 58 (82.9%) had closed canopies, and 12 (17.1%) had open canopies. The sites with young forest showed a very similar distribution of canopy cover, with 115 (78.2%) of the sites having closed canopies and 32 (21.8%) having open canopies. Canopy cover was not correlated with age of the forest (chi-square test, χ² = 0.38, n = 217, P > 0.54). The territories of adult robins were not randomly distributed within the island’s forest habitats. As seen in Fig. 3, old forests were used more than expected, and young forest areas were avoided (likelihood ratio test, χ² = 40.9, n = 217, P < 0.001). Adult robins also tended to have their territories in areas with more canopy cover within the patches (likelihood ratio test, χ² = 3.48, n = 281, P = 0.06). In contrast to this, juveniles did not show differences in their habitat use (likelihood ratio test, forest age, χ² = 1.5, n = 217, P = 0.28, canopy cover, χ² = 0.001, n = 281, P = 0.98). Connectivity and patch characteristics Our results suggest that habitat isolation affects patterns of natal dispersal in the North Island robin (Table 1). More than 50% of the juveniles were observed in patches with a mean patch distance of less than 20 m, and only 10.4% of the juveniles were found in patches that were isolated by more than 60 m (Fig. 2B). The probability of juveniles visiting a patch was also affected by patch size, and the number of juveniles visiting the patch increased with increasing patch size (Table 1, Fig. 2A). This suggests that the dispersal of North Island robin juveniles may be highly affected by fragmentation. Our results support earlier suggestions by O’Donnell (1991) and Higgins and Peter (2003) that robins are reluctant to cross open areas between habitat patches. Adult North Island robins showed a preference for establishing breeding territories in old forest and also preferred areas with more canopy cover (Fig. 3). This supports previous observations that adult North Island robins are predominantly found in older forests and that forest age is an indicator of habitat quality for this species (O’Donnell 1991, Spurr et al. 1992, Higgins and Peter 2003). Additionally, the assumptions of the importance of canopy cover for reduced adult and chick mortality from predation and bad weather are supported by the observed preference of territory establishment in sites with more canopy cover. During dispersal, juveniles should settle in habitats that are similar to their predispersal habitat, a process called “habitat training” (Stamps 2001). This is to save energy resources for reproduction that would otherwise be used for learning how to live in a new kind of habitat; thus adult and juvenile North Island robins should be found in similar habitats. However, we found no habitat associations for juvenile birds during the post-fledging dispersal (see also Miller et al. 1997, Anders et al. 1998). One reason for this is that normal measures of habitat quality for robins, i.e., forest age, are less important than areas that provide food and shelter from predators. A preference for old forest was found when examining the habitat preference of juveniles at the landscape scale, i.e., the patches to which the juveniles were moving (Table 1, Fig. 2B). This difference between habitat selection at the landscape scale and selection at the smaller scale within the patches could be explained by juveniles searching for high-quality patches suitable for breeding, but being unable to settle in high-quality areas within these areas because of high rates of aggression from resident adults (Lambin et al. 2001, Berggren et al. 2004, Doligez et al. 2004). As a result, juveniles must search for unoccupied areas in: (1) unoccupied and low-quality patches; (2) edge areas of high-quality patches, which often consist of young forest; (3) areas between existing territories, which can consist of either old or young forest; or (4) an area left temporarily vacant as a result of the death of the territory owner. Juveniles that left their natal area earlier in the season occupied patches that were less isolated and more popular among juveniles, i.e., with more juveniles visiting the patch, than juveniles leaving later. High competition for space between both adults and older juveniles may force later-arriving juveniles to choose patches they would normally avoid. Thus, the distribution of robins on Tiritiri Matangi Island more or less follows the predictions for an ideal despotic distribution (Calsbeek and Sinervo 2002), assuming that juveniles that left their natal habitat later in the season are younger and thus less experienced in competing for habitat than any older birds. This could negatively influence the survival for late dispersers in terms of higher predation risk and less food availability in nonpreferred areas. Although we acknowledge that the sample size of late-dispersing juveniles is low (n = 6), we believe that the results are valid and may partly explain the density-dependent juvenile survival suggested by Armstrong et al. (2002, see also Arcese et al. 1992, Stamps 2001). The North Island robin is a highly threatened, forest-living bird species that is sedentary in its territory throughout the year after the initial phase of natal dispersal (Higgins and Peter 2003). Much of New Zealand has been deforested, and many of the remaining lowland forests are fragmented and isolated (Myers 1923, McGlone et al. 2005). Our study indicates that the dispersal of the North Island robin is highly affected by fragmentation and that the species is reluctant to cross gaps (see also Armstrong et al. 2000, Berggren et al. 2004), a factor likely to be more pronounced in sedentary bird species like the North Island robin (Saunders et al. 1991, Andrén 1994, Sieving et al. 2000, Hames et al. 2001). One method suggested for reducing the extinction risk of species with poor dispersal abilities is the preservation or creation of habitat corridors linking the remaining habitat (Meffe and Caroll 1994, Macdonald and Johnson 2001, Berggren et al. 2002, Haddad et al. 2003). Vegetation strips (corridors) or small fragments (stepping stones) between larger habitat areas increase landscape connectivity and, thus, may increase the movement of individuals between the isolated fragments (Ims 1995, Tischendorf and Fahrig 2000). In addition, wide corridors offer additional habitat that can be used for breeding (Merriam 1991). The birds most likely to benefit from corridors are forest-habitat specialists: those species with low mobility in open habitats and of a high conservation status (O’Donnell 1991). New Zealand robins have been listed as one of eight native birds of that class in New Zealand, and one of the species most likely to decline after any form of habitat modification (O’Donnell 1991, Spurr et al. 1992). The suggested fragmentation sensitivity of the robin in combination with the results from our study indicates that any improvement of the landscape to increase connectivity would benefit the long-term survival of the species. We found dispersing juvenile North Island robins in all forest habitats, including young and open forest areas, i.e., poor breeding habitat. This suggests that juvenile robins make dispersal decisions on a broader habitat scale and are not dependent on the older forest stands during their dispersal phase. Thus, landscape connectivity could be increased for robins during natal dispersal by restoring low-quality forest patches between higher quality patches (Wiegand et al. 2005). Increased connectivity between forest patches would also be expected to increase the successful dispersal of other threatened native species (Vols et al. 2002). The relationship between number of visiting juveniles and patch size may indicate that the benefit of a corridor or stepping stones would increase with increasing size of the element. There may even be a threshold below which the element is not used. To estimate the dispersal behavior of the North Island robin in corridors and stepping stones, more studies are needed. There is a risk with corridors that they can have detrimental effects for the individuals traveling through them, because they may provide dispersal abilities not only for threatened species but also for noxious agents such as fire, disease, or pests (Simberloff et al. 1992). Because of the high fraction of edge habitat in corridors, they might further provide habitat for edge-inhabiting predators and thus act as a sink for the animals using them. However, the improvement of dispersal in a target species living in a highly fragmented landscape is likely to far outweigh any potential negative effects. Dispersal through corridors or other types of structural links between habitats would give individuals in a population an escape route when conditions deteriorate, which would reduce the risk of inbreeding and extinction of the population (Saunders and Hobbs 1991). Understanding dispersal in a species like the North Island robin that lives in a highly fragmented habitat is essential for future conservation and management. With such knowledge it is possible to change the management of both the species in focus and the landscape it inhabits so that individuals can recolonize former habitats and, thus, increase their long-term survival. More studies are needed on the effect of re-created habitats on individual dispersal in this species, but the combination of earlier studies and the findings from this study suggest that efforts made to increase the connectivity between habitats used by the North Island robin may be effective in increasing the dispersal and longevity of the populations. Responses to this article are invited. If accepted for publication, your response will be hyperlinked to the article. To submit a response, follow this link. To read responses already accepted, follow this link ACKNOWLEDGMENTSWe would like to thank Ray and Barbara Walter and Ian Price, the Department of Conservation rangers on Tiritiri Matangi Island who assisted us so kindly. Ray Walter also helped us by supplying vegetation maps with replanting data. Several others lent us a helping hand whenever it was necessary: thanks to Elna Broeder, Wendy Dimond, Doug Armstrong, Sandra Jack, Matthew Low, Staffan Roos, and Laura Mae Young. We also thank Staffan Roos, Matthew Low, Tomas Pärt, and Bo Söderström for comments on statistics and earlier versions of the paper. The Supporters of Tiritiri Matangi Island and the Department of Conservation made it possible for us to do this research by letting us use the facilities on Tiritiri Matangi Island. The research was supported by grants from the New Zealand Lottery Grants Board and the Supporters of Tiritiri Matangi Inc. The project was conducted under a research permit from the New Zealand Department of Conservation. Anders, A. D., J. Faaborg, and F. R. Thompson. 1998. Postfledging dispersal, habitat use, and home-range size of juvenile wood trushes. The Auk 115: 349-358. Andrén, H. 1994. Effects of habitat fragmentation on birds and mammals in landscapes with different proportions of suitable habitat: a review. Oikos 71:355-366. Arcese, P., J. N. M. Smith, W. H. Hochaka, C. M. Rogers, and D. Ludwig. 1992. Stability, regulation, and the determination of abundance in an insular song sparrow population. Ecology 73:805-822. Armstrong, D. P. 1995. Effects of familiarity on the outcome of translocations. II. A test using New Zealand robins. Biological Conservation 71:281-288. Armstrong, D. P., R. S. Davidson, W. J. Dimond, J. K. Perrott, L. Castro, J. G. Ewen, R. Griffiths, and J. Taylor. 2002. Population dynamics of reintroduced forest birds on New Zealand islands. Journal of Biogeography 29:609-621. Armstrong, D. P., and J. G. Ewen. 2002. Dynamics and viability on a New Zealand robin population reintroduced to regenerating fragmented habitat. Conservation Biology 16:1074-1085. Armstrong, D. P., J. G. Ewen, W. J. Dimond, T. G. Lovegrove, Å. Bergström, and B. Walter. 2000. Breeding biology of North Island robins (Petroica australis longipes) on Tiritiri Matangi Island, Hauraki Gulf, New Zealand. Notornis 47:106-118. Baker-Gabb, D. J. 1981. The diet of the Australasian harrier (Circus approximans) in the Manawatu-Rangitikei sand country, New Zealand. Notornis 28:241-254. Barbosa, O., and P. A. Marquet. 2002. Effects of fragmentation on the beetle assemblage at the relict forest of Fray Jorge, Chile. Oecologia 132:296-306. Bender, D. J., T. A. Contreras, and L. Fahrig. 1998. Habitat loss and population decline: a meta-analysis of the patch size effect. Ecology 79:517-533. Berggren, Å. 2006. Intraspecific adoption and foster feeding of fledglings in the North Island robin. New Zealand Journal of Ecology 30:209-217. Berggren, Å., D. P. Armstrong, and R. M. Lewis. 2004. Delayed plumage maturation increases overwinter survival in North Island robins. Proceedings of the Royal Science of London B 271:2123-2130. Berggren, Å., B. Birath, and O. Kindvall. 2002. Effect of corridors and habitat edges on dispersal behaviour, movement rates, and movement angles in Roesel’s Bush-Cricket (Metrioptera roeseli). Conservation Biology 16:1562-1569. Brown, K. P., H. Moller, J. Innes, and P. Jansen. 1998. Identifying predators at nests of small birds in a New Zealand forest. Ibis 140:274-279. Calsbeek, R., and B. Sinervo. 2002. An experimental test of the ideal despotic distribution. Journal of Animal Ecology 71: 513-523. Charlesworth, D., and B. Charlesworth. 1987. Inbreeding depression and its evolutionary consequences. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 18:237-268. Doligez, B., T. Pärt, E. Danchin, J. Clobert, and L. Gustafsson. 2004. Availability and use of public information and conspecific density for settlement decisions in the collared flycatcher. Journal of Animal Ecology 73:75-87. Fleishman, E., C. Ray, P. Sjögren-Gulve, C. L. Boggs, and D. D. Murphy. 2002. Assessing the role of patch quality, area and isolation in predicting metapopulation dynamics. Conservation Biology 16:706-716. Fleming, C. A. 1950. New Zealand flycatchers of the genus Petroica Swainson (Aves). Transactions of the Royal Society of New Zealand 78:127-160. Haddad, N. M., D. R. Bowne, A. Cunningham, B. J. Danielson, D. J. Levey, S. Sargent, and T. Spira. 2003. Corridor use by diverse taxa. Ecology 84:609-615. Hames, R. S., K. V. Rosenberg, J. D. Lowe, and A. A. Dhondt. 2001. Site reoccupation in fragmented landscapes: testing predictions of metapopulation theory. Journal of Animal Ecology 70:182-190. Hanski, I. 1999. Metapopulation ecology. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK. Hansson, L. 1991. Dispersal connectivity in metapopulations. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 42:89-103. Higgins, P. J., and J. M. Peter. 2003. Handbook of the Australian, New Zealand and Antarctic birds. Sixth edition. Oxford University Press, Melbourne, Australia. Holdaway, R. N., T. H. Worthy, and A. J. D. Tennyson. 2001. A working list of breeding bird species in the New Zealand region at first human contact. New Zealand Journal of Zoology 28:119-187. Ims, R. A. 1995. Movement patterns related to spatial structures. Pages 85-109 in L. Hannson, L. Fahrig, and G. Merriam, editors. Mosaic landscapes and ecological processes. Chapman & Hall, London, UK. Innes, J., G. Nugent, K. Prime, and E. B. Spurr. 2004. Responses of kukupa (Hemiphaga novaeseelandiae) and other birds to mammal pest control at Motatau, Northland. New Zealand Journal of Ecology 28:73-81. Johnson, M. L., and M. S. Gaines. 1990. Evolution of dispersal: theoretical models and empirical tests using birds and mammals. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 21:449-480. Kennedy, P., and J. M. Ward. 2003. Effects of experimental food supply on movements of juvenile northern goshawks (Accipiter gentiles atricapillus). Oecologia 134:284-291. Lambin, X., J. Aars, and S. B. Piertney. 2001. Dispersal, intraspecific competition, kin competition and kin faciliation: a review of the empirical evidence. Pages 110-122 in J. Clobert, E. Danchin, A. A. Dondt, and J. D. Nichols, editors. Dispersal. Oxford University Press, New York, New York, USA. Macdonald, D. W., and D. D. P. Johnson. 2001. Dispersal in theory and practice: consequences for conservation biology. Pages 358-372 in J. Clobert, E. Danchin, A. A. Dondt, and J. D. Nichols, editors. Dispersal. Oxford University Press, New York, New York, USA. Matthysen, E. 2005. Density-dependent dispersal in birds and mammals. Ecography 28:403-416. McCollagh, P. 1980. Regression models for ordinal data. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series B 42:109-142. McGlone, M. S., J. M. Wilmshurst, and M. Leach. 2005. An ecological and historical review of bracken (Pteridium esculentum) in New Zealand, and its cultural significance. New Zealand Journal of Ecology 29:165-184. McLennan, J. A., and M. A. Potter. 1992. Distribution, population changes and management of brown kiwi in Hawke’s Bay. New Zealand Journal of Ecology 16:91-102. Meffe, G. K., and C. P. Carroll. 1994. Principles of conservation biology. Sinauer, Sunderland, Massachusetts, USA. Merriam, G. 1991. Corridors and connectivity: animal populations in heterogenous environments. Pages 133-142 in D. A. Saunders and R. J. Hobbs, editors. Nature conservation 2: the role of corridors. Surrey Beatty, Chipping Norton, Australia. . Miller, G. S., R. J. Small, and E. C. Meslow. 1997. Habitat selection by spotted owls during natal dispersal in western Oregon. Journal of Wildlife Management 61:140-150. Myers, J. G. 1923. The present position of the endemic birds of New Zealand. New Zealand Journal of Science and Technology 6:65-99. O’Donnell, C. F. J. 1991. Application of the wildlife corridors concept to temperate rainforest sites, North Westland, New Zealand. Pages 85-98 in D. A. Saunders and R. J. Hobbs, editors. Nature conservation 2: the role of corridors. Surrey Beatty, Chipping Norton, Australia. Opdam, P. 1991. Metapopulation theory and habitat fragmentation: a review of holarctic breeding bird studies. Landscape Ecology 5:93-106. Oppel, S., and B. Beaven. 2004. Juvenile Stewart Island robins (Petroica australis rakiura) disperse up to 16 km; short note. Notornis 51:55-56. Ozi-Explorer. 2000. Software version 3.90.3. D & L Software, Brisbane, Australia. Perrin, N., and G. Goudet. 2001. Inbreeding, kinship, and the evolution of natal dispersal. Pages 123-142 in J. Clobert, E. Danchin, A. A. Dondt, and J. D. Nichols, editors. Dispersal. Oxford University Press, New York, New York, USA. SAS Institute. 1995. JMP version 3. SAS Institute, Cary, North Carolina, USA. Saunders, D. A., and R. J. Hobbs. 1991. The role of corridors. Surrey Beatty, Chipping Norton, Australia. Saunders, D. A., R. J. Hobbs, and C. R. Margules. 1991. Biological consequences of ecosystem fragmentation: a review. Conservation Biology 5:18-32. Sieving, K. E., M. F. Willson, and T. L. de Santo. 2000. Defining corridor functions for endemic birds in fragmented south-temperate rainforest. Conservation Biology 14:1120-1132. Simberloff, D., J. A. Farr, J. Cox, and D. W. Mehlman. 1992. Movement corridors: conservation bargains or poor investments? Conservation Biology 6:493-504. Smith, J. N. M., and J. J. Hellmann. 2002. Population persistence in fragmented landscapes. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 17:397-399. Spurr, E. B., B. Warburton, and K. W. Drew. 1992. Bird abundance in different-aged stands of Rimu (Dacrydium curpressinum)—implications for coupe-logging. New Zealand Journal of Ecology 16:109-118. Stamps, J. A. 2001. Habitat selection by dispersers: integrating proximate and ultimate approaches. Pages 230-242 in J. Clobert, E. Danchin, A. A. Dondt, and J. D. Nichols, editors. Dispersal. Oxford University Press, New York, New York, USA. Sutherland, W. J., J. A. Gill, and K. Norris. 2002. Density-dependent dispersal in animals: concepts, evidence, mechanisms and consequences. Pages 134-151 in J. M. Bullock, R. E. Kenward, and R. S. Hails, editors. Dispersal ecology. Blackwell, Oxford, UK. Thomson, D. L., R. W. Furness, and P. Monaghan. 1998. The analysis of ordinal response data in the behavioural sciences. Animal Behaviour 56: 1041-1043. Tischendorf, L., and L. Fahrig. 2000. On the usage and measurement of landscape connectivity. Oikos 90:7-18. Travis, J. M. J., and C. Dytham. 1998. The evolution of dispersal in a metapopulation: a spatially explicit, individual-based model. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences 265:17-23 Vols, C. C., H. Baveco, and C. J. Grashof-Bokdam. 2002. Corridors and species dispersal. Pages 84-103 in K. J. Gutzwiller, editor. Applying landscape ecology in biological conservation. Springer Verlag, New York, New York, USA. Warburton, B., S. J. Kingsford, D. W. Lewitt, and E. B. Spurr. 1992. Plant species preferences of birds in lowland rimu (Dacrydium cupressinum) forest—implications for selective logging. New Zealand Journal of Ecology 16:119-126. Wiegand, T., E. Revilla, and K. A. Moloney. 2005. Effects of habitat loss and fragmentation on population dynamics. Conservation Biology 19:108-121. Withey, J. C., and J. M. Marzluff. 2005. Dispersal by juvenile American crows (Corvus brachyrhynchus) influences population dynamics across a gradient of urbanization. The Auk 122:205-221.
|
||||
Home | Archives | About | Login | Submissions | Notify | Contact | Search | ||||